We are currently in the process of undertaking a systematic literature review of existing vocabulary interventions carried out with children who have Down syndrome. Having a good insight into existing interventions in this area is very useful for us when planning our DSL+-intervention. As we work on this review we will write a post each week about a different vocabulary intervention article discovered through the review process.
The first article that we are going to present in this series of posts is by Burgoyne, Duff, Clarke, Buckley, Snowling, & Hulme (2012); they provided evidence that a combined reading and language intervention was beneficial for vocabulary development (as well as literacy development) in children with Down syndrome. This study, which is one of the very few Randomised Control Trial (RCT) studies within language intervention for children with Down syndrome, involved 57 children (primary school years 1–5) and was delivered by their teaching assistants who received training on how to implement the intervention sessions.
The experimental
group received training every day in school and each session was 40 minutes
long. A waiting control-group receive the intervention at a later point.
Various
measures related to reading and language were completed by the children at four
different time points: 1. At screening, 2. Immediately prior to intervention,
3. After 20 weeks of intervention, and 4. After 40 weeks of intervention. Each
week children received 4 sessions on ‘new teaching’ and one session dedicated
to revision. The reading strand of the intervention focused on teaching reading
and phonics together, based on Hatcher, Hulme and Ellis (1994). The language
strand of the intervention focused on teaching children new vocabulary items;
aiming to result in children expressing these new words. The new words were
chosen based on a set of parent checklists to determine which words many of the
children did not already know or understand and therefore would be useful to
the children in general. Visual support and various games e.g., matching, were
used to teach the new words.
After the
first 20 weeks of individual daily intervention, the experimental group showed
significantly greater improvement compared to the waiting control group on
measures of taught expressive vocabulary, single word reading, letter-sound
knowledge, and phoneme blending. These
measures were all proximal to the content of the intervention. No transfer
effects to any other language related skills measured were found. After 40
weeks of intervention for the experimental group and 20 weeks of intervention
for the waiting controls, the experimental group was still ahead of the control
group on most outcome measures, but the differences between the groups were not
statistically significant.
The study by
Burgoyne and colleagues was well designed, using RCT, obtaining a larger sample
size than other studies in this area, and providing a long training period. Nonetheless,
some limitations in the study and results are also present. As the authors
note, the effect size was very modest. Another point to note is the age spread
of the sample; how a 5 year old with Down syndrome may respond to language and
reading intervention may be very different of course to the way that a 10 year
old with Down syndrome may respond to such intervention. Looking at
correlations, Burgoyne et al. found that across group’s younger children
experienced greater reading growth by the end of the 40 weeks. Having an
age-spread sample makes it difficult to know what outcome and effect size to
expect from a given intervention at a certain point in children’s development.
Of course, this is a tricky issue, as achieving a large sample is important (e.g.
due to power in the analysis), and due to various practicalities researchers
may have to make a compromise between sample size vs age range.
Potential limitations may also relate to teaching assistants delivering the intervention; specifically, their opportunities for preparation time and training, such as in pedagogical knowledge and how this impacts intervention implementation. Webster et al. (2011) notes that there is often an ‘instinctive, but mistaken assumption that less pedagogical skill is required when teaching pupils with SEN; if anything, a higher level of skill is needed’ (pg. 15). Those teaching assistants who have a pedagogical background appear to ensure better implementation of training, resulting in greater results (Alborz et al., 2009).
Finally,
because the program included both vocabulary and reading components, it is not
possible to conclude whether it is the vocabulary training or the reading that
is critical or if it is the combined approach that results in the observed effect.
The intervention resulted in greater reading gains relative to language gains.
It would be interesting to explore the effects of only the vocabulary training
or only the reading training components in a comparison group, to see how this
impacts the same outcome measures.
To
summarize, this RCT study by Burgoyne and colleagues had many strengths and did
improve reading and language outcomes (including vocabulary) in children with
Down syndrome. As is so often the case in training studies however, the effects
of the intervention did not generalize to other aspects of reading and language
more distal from the intervention content. As with any study, there are some
issues to consider regarding these outcomes.
Look out for
our second post next week discussing another vocabulary intervention article carried
out with children who have Down syndrome.
References:
Alborz, A., Pearson, D., Farrell, P., &
Howes, A. (2009). The impact of adult support staff on pupils and mainstream
schools. London: Department for Children, Schools and Families and Institute of
Education.
Burgoyne,
K., Duff, F. J., Clarke, P. J., Buckley, S., Snowling, M. J., & Hulme, C.
(2012). Efficacy of a reading and language intervention for children with Down
syndrome: a randomized controlled trial. Journal
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(10),
1044-1053.
Hatcher,
P. J., Hulme, C., & Ellis, A. W. (1994). Ameliorating early reading failure
by integrating the teaching of reading and phonological skills: The
phonological linkage hypothesis. Child
development, 65(1),
41-57.
Webster,
R., Blatchford, P., Bassett, P., Brown, P., Martin, C., & Russell, A.
(2011). The wider pedagogical role of teaching assistants. School Leadership and Management, 31(1), 3-20.
-Liz Smith
and Kari-Anne B. Næss-
Ingen kommentarer:
Legg inn en kommentar